Updated: 2025-08-10
The heritability of intelligence is a complex topic that has been extensively studied through twin, adoption, and molecular genetic research. Current scientific evidence suggests that genetic factors account for approximately 50-80% of the variation in intelligence among individuals, though this estimate varies by age and methodology.
Research consistently shows that the heritability of intelligence changes across the lifespan. In early childhood, genetic factors account for about 20-40% of intelligence differences, but this proportion increases steadily with age [4]. By adolescence, heritability rises to approximately 50-60%, and in adulthood, it can reach 70-80% [4][7]. This phenomenon, known as the Wilson Effect, suggests that genetic influences on intelligence become more pronounced as individuals mature and have greater control over their environments.
A meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies found that heritability increases from about 41% in childhood to 55% in adolescence and 66% in young adulthood [4]. This pattern has been replicated across multiple studies and populations, indicating a robust developmental trend.
Recent advances in genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have identified specific genetic variants associated with intelligence. A large meta-analysis involving 269,867 individuals identified 205 independent genetic loci significantly associated with intelligence [3]. These studies have shown that intelligence is a highly polygenic trait, meaning it is influenced by thousands of genetic variants, each with small individual effects [2][7].
However, there remains a gap between heritability estimates from twin studies (50-80%) and the variance explained by identified genetic variants (currently about 10-20%) [5][6]. This “missing heritability” problem suggests that either many genetic variants with small effects remain undiscovered, or that other factors such as rare variants, gene-gene interactions, or epigenetic mechanisms play important roles.
Recent within-family studies, which compare siblings who share approximately 50% of their DNA, provide particularly robust evidence for genetic influences on intelligence. These studies help control for environmental factors that families share [8]. Polygenic scores (which aggregate the effects of many genetic variants) can predict intelligence differences even between siblings raised in the same household, though the effect sizes are smaller than in population-wide studies [8].
While genetics plays a substantial role, environmental factors remain crucial for intellectual development. The remaining 20-50% of variance in intelligence is attributed to environmental influences, including education, nutrition, socioeconomic status, and early childhood experiences [1][2]. Importantly, genetic and environmental factors are not independent but interact in complex ways. For example, genetic predispositions may influence the environments individuals select or create for themselves, a phenomenon known as gene-environment correlation [7].
Several important limitations should be considered when interpreting heritability estimates:
Population-specific: Heritability estimates apply to populations, not individuals, and can vary across different populations and environments [1][5].
Not fixed: Heritability can change if environmental conditions change substantially. In more equitable environments with reduced environmental variation, heritability tends to be higher [2].
Doesn’t imply immutability: High heritability does not mean intelligence cannot be influenced by interventions or environmental changes [1][6].
Complex trait: Intelligence is not determined by a single gene but involves thousands of genetic variants and their interactions with environmental factors [3][5].
Current scientific evidence indicates that intelligence has a substantial genetic component, with heritability estimates ranging from 50-80% in adults. However, this does not diminish the importance of environmental factors, which account for 20-50% of the variation and interact with genetic factors in complex ways. The field continues to advance through large-scale genetic studies and sophisticated statistical methods, gradually uncovering the biological mechanisms underlying cognitive abilities while acknowledging the crucial role of environmental influences.
Is Intelligence Hereditary? – Scientific American - Discusses the complexity of intelligence heritability, emphasizing that while genetics plays a role (estimates around 50%), environmental factors are equally important, and heritability doesn’t mean determinism.
The new genetics of intelligence – Nature Reviews Genetics - Comprehensive review showing intelligence is highly polygenic with heritability around 50-80%, discussing GWAS findings and gene-environment interactions.
Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence – Nature Genetics - Large-scale GWAS identifying 205 genetic loci associated with intelligence, demonstrating the polygenic nature of cognitive abilities.
Explaining the Increasing Heritability of Cognitive Ability Across Development – Psychological Science - Meta-analysis showing heritability increases from 41% in childhood to 66% in young adulthood, supporting the Wilson Effect.
Genetic Variation, Brain, and Intelligence Differences – Molecular Psychiatry - Reviews neurobiological mechanisms linking genetic variation to intelligence, discussing brain structure and function correlates.
DNA and IQ: Big Deal or Much Ado About Nothing? – A Meta-analysis – Intelligence - Meta-analysis examining the relationship between DNA-based measures and IQ, addressing the missing heritability problem.
The new genetics of intelligence – Nature Reviews Genetics (PMC Open Access) - Open access version providing detailed discussion of polygenic scores, heritability estimates of 50-80%, and future directions in genetic research.
Polygenic Score Prediction Within And Between Sibling Pairs – Intelligence & Cognitive Abilities Journal - Within-family study demonstrating genetic prediction of intelligence differences between siblings, providing robust evidence controlling for shared environment.
Short answer
What “heritable” means (and does not mean)
Evidence 1) Family-based studies (twins, adoptees)
2) DNA-based estimates and predictions
Why heritability rises with age
Important caveats
Bottom line
Sources 1) Is Intelligence Hereditary? – Scientific American. Overview for general readers; concludes intelligence is substantially heritable (often around 50% in adults) but strongly shaped by environment and context; clarifies what heritability does and does not mean. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/is-intelligence-hereditary/
2) The new genetics of intelligence – Nature Reviews Genetics (PMC Open Access). Scholarly review summarizing twin-based heritability (~50–70% in adults), rising heritability across development, SNP-heritability (~20–30%), polygenicity, and limitations/ethical issues; emphasizes gene–environment interplay and improving DNA prediction. https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC5985927/
3) Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence – Nature Genetics. Large GWAS identifying many loci; reports common-variant (SNP) heritability and that polygenic scores explain a modest share of variance, confirming substantial but partial genetic architecture for intelligence. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29942086/
4) Explaining the Increasing Heritability of Cognitive Ability Across Development: A Meta-analysis of Longitudinal Twin and Adoption Studies – Psychological Science. Meta-analysis showing heritability of cognitive ability rises from low in infancy to substantially higher in adolescence/adulthood, with declining shared-environment effects; highlights mechanisms like gene–environment correlation. https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC3954471/
5) Genetic Variation, Brain, and Intelligence Differences – Molecular Psychiatry. Review linking genetic variation to brain measures and intelligence; reiterates high polygenicity, twin-based heritability around half or more in adults, and SNP-heritability around a quarter; discusses biological pathways and measurement issues. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41380-021-01027-y
6) DNA and IQ: Big Deal or Much Ado About Nothing? – A Meta-analysis – Intelligence. Meta-analytic assessment of DNA-based prediction; concludes polygenic scores for IQ/cognitive traits capture a meaningful but limited portion of variance (generally in the single to low double digits), far from determinism. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0160289624000655
7) Polygenic Score Prediction Within And Between Sibling Pairs For Intelligence Cognitive Abilities And Educational Traits From Childhood To Early Adulthood – Intelligence & Cognitive Abilities Journal. Shows PGS predict cognitive and educational outcomes both between unrelated individuals and within families (attenuated within-family), supporting genuine genetic effects alongside environmental influences. https://icajournal.scholasticahq.com/article/140654-polygenic-score-prediction-within-and-between-sibling-pairs-for-intelligence-cognitive-abilities-and-educational-traits-from-childhood-to-early-adul
8) Stability of general cognitive ability from infancy to adulthood – PNAS. Documents meaningful rank-order stability of general cognitive ability across the lifespan, consistent with developmental processes that allow genetic influences to accumulate even as environments matter. https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.2426531122
9) The new genetics of intelligence – Nature Reviews Genetics (paywalled version). Same review as [2]; emphasizes polygenicity, developmental increase in heritability, and advances in genomic prediction and methods. https://www.nature.com/articles/nrg.2017.104
10) Three promising directions in the study of intelligence with genetic methods – GWAS Intelligence (preprint). Perspective piece outlining methodological advances (e.g., within-family, causal inference, diverse samples) to refine estimates and interpretability of genetic influences on intelligence. https://jamesjlee.altervista.org/wp-content/uploads/2025/04/genetics_intelligence_CDPS.pdf
Based on decades of research, intelligence is one of the most heritable psychological traits studied [1, 2]. Heritability is a statistical measure that estimates the proportion of differences in a trait among individuals in a population that can be attributed to genetic differences [1]. It does not indicate how much of a single individual’s intelligence is due to their genes. The scientific consensus is that genetic factors play a substantial role in the variation of intelligence, but the exact contribution and mechanisms are complex and change over a person’s lifetime [2, 4].
For many years, the primary evidence for the heritability of intelligence came from studies of twins, adoptees, and families. These studies consistently show that genetics are a major contributor to intelligence differences [1, 2].
Modern genetics, particularly through Genome-Wide Association Studies (GWAS), has moved from estimating heritability to identifying the specific genetic variants involved [2].
Using the results from GWAS, scientists can create a “polygenic score” (PGS) for an individual, which aggregates the many small genetic effects to estimate their genetic predisposition for intelligence [8].
In summary, a large proportion of the variation in intelligence within a population is due to genetic factors. Heritability is substantial, increasing from childhood to adulthood to as high as 80% [4]. Modern research has identified thousands of specific genes that contribute to these differences [3]. While the environment is undeniably crucial, genetic predispositions play a major role in shaping cognitive ability [1, 2].
Intelligence is a complex trait influenced by both genetic and environmental factors. The question of how much of it is hereditary often refers to heritability, a statistical concept that estimates the proportion of variation in intelligence (typically measured by IQ or cognitive ability tests) within a population that can be attributed to genetic differences among individuals. Importantly, heritability does not mean that intelligence is fixed or predetermined; it reflects the relative contribution of genetics in a specific environment and population, and it can change across contexts or lifespans. It also does not imply that the remaining portion is solely environmental, as gene-environment interactions play a role. Based on extensive research from twin studies, adoption studies, and modern genomic analyses, heritability estimates for intelligence generally range from 40% to 80%, with variations depending on age, measurement methods, and populations studied [1][2][4]. Below, I break this down comprehensively, drawing on the suggested sources and a few additional high-quality references for completeness and accuracy.
Traditional studies, such as those involving twins and adoptees, provide the foundational evidence for intelligence’s heritability. These compare identical (monozygotic) twins, who share nearly 100% of their DNA, with fraternal (dizygotic) twins, who share about 50%, or compare adopted children with their biological and adoptive parents.
Overall Heritability Range: Meta-analyses of twin and family studies suggest that genetics account for approximately 50% of the variance in intelligence in adulthood, though estimates can reach as high as 80% in some populations [1][2]. For instance, a comprehensive review in Nature Reviews Genetics highlights that heritability increases from about 20-40% in early childhood to 60-80% in adulthood, a phenomenon known as the “Wilson effect” where genetic influences become more pronounced as individuals age and shape their own environments [2][4].
Age-Related Changes: Longitudinal studies confirm that heritability is lower in infancy and early childhood (around 20-30%) but rises steadily. By adolescence, it often exceeds 50%, and in adulthood, it can approach 80% [4][9]. This increase is attributed to gene-environment correlations, where genetically influenced traits lead individuals to seek out environments that amplify those traits (e.g., a child with high genetic potential for cognition might pursue more intellectually stimulating activities) [4][10].
These estimates are population-specific and can vary by socioeconomic status or cultural context. For example, in environments with high inequality, environmental factors may play a larger role, reducing apparent heritability [1].
Advances in genome-wide association studies (GWAS) and polygenic scoring have moved beyond broad heritability estimates to identify specific genetic variants associated with intelligence. These methods scan millions of DNA markers to find correlations with cognitive traits.
GWAS Findings: A large-scale meta-analysis of over 269,000 individuals identified hundreds of genetic loci linked to intelligence, explaining about 5-10% of the variance in cognitive ability [3]. More recent reviews suggest that polygenic scores—composite measures of many small genetic effects—can predict up to 10-15% of intelligence differences within populations [5][6][7]. However, this is far below the 50-80% from twin studies, indicating a “missing heritability” gap, where unidentified genetic factors or complex interactions may account for the rest [5][10].
Within-Family Evidence: Studies comparing siblings (who share family environments but differ genetically) show that polygenic scores predict intelligence differences, supporting a causal genetic role. For example, research on sibling pairs found that genetic scores explain about 4-7% of variance in cognitive abilities from childhood to adulthood, with stability over time [8]. This helps rule out confounding environmental factors like shared upbringing [8][10].
Limitations and the “Big Deal or Much Ado” Debate: While GWAS have identified real genetic links, critics argue the predictive power is modest and overhyped. A meta-analysis in Intelligence concluded that DNA-based predictions for IQ are statistically significant but explain only a small fraction (around 5-10%) of variance, cautioning against overinterpreting them for individual predictions [6]. Environmental influences, such as education, nutrition, and socioeconomic status, remain crucial and can interact with genetics (e.g., via epigenetics) [1][2].
Heritability is not destiny; even with high genetic contributions, interventions like quality education can significantly boost cognitive outcomes [1]. Intelligence is polygenic, involving thousands of genes each with tiny effects, and it’s influenced by rare mutations or de novo variants [5][7]. Moreover, definitions of “intelligence” vary (e.g., general cognitive ability vs. specific skills), affecting estimates [9]. Cross-cultural studies suggest heritability may be lower in resource-poor environments, emphasizing the interplay between genes and surroundings [2].
In summary, while genetics play a substantial role—accounting for roughly 50% of intelligence variance on average, and up to 80% in adulthood—the exact figure depends on context. Molecular studies are closing the gap but highlight that much remains unexplained. For a more precise personal assessment, genetic testing is not yet reliable, and environmental factors offer significant opportunities for enhancement [1][3][6]. Additional sources, such as a review in Psychological Bulletin [11] and a book chapter on behavioral genetics [12], reinforce these findings by synthesizing global twin data and emphasizing gene-environment dynamics.
The heritability of intelligence is one of the most extensively studied topics in behavioral genetics, yet it remains complex and sometimes controversial. Research consistently shows that intelligence has a significant genetic component, though environmental factors also play a crucial role.
Twin, family, and adoption studies consistently indicate that intelligence, as measured by IQ tests, has a heritability of approximately 50-80% in adults [1]. The heritability appears to increase with age: around 20% in infancy, 40% in childhood, and reaching 60-80% in adulthood [2]. This phenomenon, known as the “Wilson Effect,” suggests that genetic influences on intelligence become more prominent as individuals mature and have greater control over their environments [3].
Meta-analyses of twin studies have provided robust evidence for genetic influence on intelligence. A comprehensive analysis of 2,748 publications examining virtually all twin studies on intelligence found a heritability estimate of approximately 50% across the lifespan [4]. Adoption studies further support these findings, showing that adopted children’s IQ scores correlate more strongly with their biological parents than their adoptive parents [1].
Recent genomic studies have identified specific genetic variants associated with intelligence, though each individual variant typically accounts for only a tiny fraction of the variance. Polygenic scores (combining effects of many genetic variants) can now predict approximately 10-15% of the variance in intelligence, with this predictive power continuing to increase as sample sizes grow [5].
While genetics plays a substantial role, environmental factors remain critically important. These include:
The heritability of intelligence varies across populations and socioeconomic contexts. In disadvantaged environments, environmental factors may play a larger role, while genetic factors become more prominent in advantaged environments where basic needs are met [7]. This is known as the Scarr-Rowe hypothesis.
It’s crucial to understand that heritability is a population statistic that describes variation within a group, not the fixed genetic determination of any individual’s intelligence [8]. High heritability does not mean intelligence is unchangeable—environmental interventions can still have significant effects.
The scientific consensus acknowledges both genetic and environmental contributions to intelligence. Most researchers agree that:
[1] Plomin, R., & von Stumm, S. (2018). The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19(3), 148-159. https://www.nature.com/articles/nrg.2017.104
[2] Sauce, B., & Matzel, L. D. (2018). The paradox of intelligence: Heritability and malleability coexist in hidden gene-environment interplay. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 26-47. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000131
[3] Briley, D. A., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2013). Explaining the increasing heritability of cognitive ability across development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 325-330. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413488620
[4] Polderman, T. J., et al. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702-709. https://www.nature.com/articles/ng.3285
[5] Allegrini, A. G., et al. (2019). Genomic prediction of cognitive traits in childhood and adolescence. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(6), 819-827. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41380-019-0394-4
[6] Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1358-1369. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618774253
[7] Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Bates, T. C. (2016). Large cross-national differences in gene Ă— socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychological Science, 27(2), 138-149. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615612727
[8] Harden, K. P. (2021). The Genetic Lottery: Why DNA Matters for Social Equality. Princeton University Press. https://press.princeton.edu/books/hardcover/9780691190808/the-genetic-lottery
[9] Nisbett, R. E., et al. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67(2), 130-159. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699
Short answer
What “heritability of intelligence” means (and does not)
What the evidence shows 1) Twin and adoption studies
2) Socioeconomic context matters
3) DNA-based (molecular) evidence
Environmental influences and malleability
Why estimates differ
Bottom line
Sources 1) Plomin, R., & von Stumm, S. (2018). The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics. View: Reviews converging evidence that intelligence is highly heritable (rising from ~20% in infancy to as high as ~80% in adulthood), polygenic, and that SNP-heritability is ~20–30%; emphasizes heritability ≠immutability. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41576-018-0012-0
2) Haworth, C. M. A., et al. (2010). The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Molecular Psychiatry. View: Meta-analysis of twin samples showing heritability increasing from ~0.41 (childhood) to ~0.66 (young adulthood) and declining shared family environment. https://www.nature.com/articles/mp2009107
3) Polderman, T. J. C., et al. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics. View: Broad synthesis showing substantial heritability for many traits; cognitive abilities show heritabilities around 50% across studies. https://www.nature.com/articles/ng.3285
4) Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Bates, T. C. (2016). Large cross-national differences in gene Ă— socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychological Science. View: Meta-analysis indicating strong GĂ—SES effects on IQ in US samples but little evidence for such effects in other developed countries. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797615612727
5) Turkheimer, E., et al. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science. View: Found lower heritability and stronger shared environmental influence on IQ among low-SES US families in early childhood. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9280.01424
6) Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science. View: Quasi-experimental evidence that additional schooling raises IQ by roughly 1–5 points per year, with lasting effects. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797618774253
7) Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A meta-analysis of the Flynn effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science. View: Documents ~3 IQ-point per decade gains across the 20th century, highlighting substantial environmental impacts on cognitive test performance. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1745691615599126
8) Duyme, M., Dumaret, A.-C., & Tomkiewicz, S. (1999). How can we boost IQs of “dull children”?: A late adoption study. PNAS. View: Children adopted from deprived backgrounds into higher-SES homes gained on average ~13 IQ points; gains scaled with quality of adoptive environment. https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.96.15.8790
9) Plomin, R., et al. (2016). Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science. View: Summarizes robust findings, including substantial heritability of intelligence and decreasing shared environmental influence with age. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1745691616652873
10) Savage, J. E., et al. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis (N=269,867) identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics. View: Shows intelligence is highly polygenic; common-variant SNP-heritability around 20% in European-ancestry cohorts; polygenic scores predict a modest share of variance. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41588-018-0152-6
11) Hill, W. D., et al. (2019). A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry. View: Replicates substantial SNP-heritability (~20–30%) and identifies many loci; supports highly polygenic architecture. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41380-018-0071-0
12) Nisbett, R. E., et al. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist. View: Reviews evidence that intelligence is substantially heritable yet highly responsive to environment; cautions against inferring group differences from heritability estimates. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0026699
The question of how much intelligence is hereditary is one of the most extensively studied and debated topics in behavioral genetics. The scientific consensus is that intelligence, as measured by cognitive tests (IQ scores), is substantially heritable, but environmental factors are also critically important. There is no single number that provides a complete answer, as the influence of genetics changes over time and interacts with the environment.
Before discussing the findings, it is crucial to understand what “heritability” means. Heritability (often denoted as h²) is a statistical concept that estimates the proportion of the observed differences (variance) in a trait among individuals within a specific population that can be attributed to genetic differences among those individuals [1, 2].
It is important to note what heritability is not:
The primary evidence for the heritability of intelligence comes from classical quantitative genetics, mainly through twin and adoption studies [3].
A meta-analysis of decades of these studies shows a consistent pattern: the heritability of intelligence is substantial. Across numerous studies, the average heritability estimate for intelligence is approximately 50% when considering results from all ages [1].
A remarkable and consistently replicated finding is that the heritability of intelligence increases with age [3]. This phenomenon is sometimes called the “Wilson Effect.”
This may seem counterintuitive, as one might expect environmental influences to accumulate over a lifetime. The leading explanation is gene-environment correlation (rGE). As individuals grow older, they gain more autonomy to select, modify, and create environments that are compatible with their genetic predispositions. For example, a child with a genetic predisposition for high verbal ability may actively seek out books, enjoy school, and pursue intellectually stimulating hobbies, thereby amplifying their initial genetic advantage [1].
A heritability estimate of 50-80% means that 20-50% of the variance in intelligence is attributable to environmental factors. These factors are typically divided into two categories:
Specific environmental factors known to impact cognitive ability include nutrition (especially iodine and iron sufficiency), exposure to toxins (like lead), and the quality and duration of education [4]. The Flynn Effect—the observed rise in IQ scores of about three points per decade throughout the 20th century in many parts of the world—is the strongest evidence for the powerful role of environmental factors, as the human gene pool could not have changed that rapidly [4].
With the advent of genomic technology, researchers can now look for specific genes associated with intelligence. Genome-Wide Association Studies (GWAS) scan the entire genome to find genetic variants (single-nucleotide polymorphisms, or SNPs) associated with a trait.
These studies have revealed that intelligence is highly polygenic, meaning it is influenced by thousands of genes, each with an extremely small effect [5]. There is no single “gene for intelligence.” Using this data, researchers can create Polygenic Scores (PGS), which aggregate the effects of thousands of identified SNPs. Currently, these scores can account for over 10% of the variance in educational attainment and intelligence, a figure that is expected to grow as sample sizes in GWAS studies increase [1, 5]. This confirms the findings of twin studies but also highlights the complexity of the genetic architecture of intelligence.
The “nature vs. nurture” debate is now understood to be an oversimplification. Genes and environment do not just add up; they interact. Gene-Environment Interaction (GxE) occurs when the effect of the environment on a trait depends on a person’s genotype, or conversely, when the effect of a genotype depends on the environment [6].
For example, some studies suggest that the heritability of intelligence is lower in families of low socioeconomic status (SES) and higher in families of high SES. This implies that a supportive and enriched environment may be necessary for individuals to fully realize their genetic potential for cognitive ability. In a deprived environment, genetic differences may have less opportunity to manifest [6].
In summary, the most accurate answer to “How much of intelligence is hereditary?” is that genetics play a major role, but their influence is not static or deterministic.
https://www.nature.com/articles/nrg.2017.104
https://www.nature.com/articles/nrg2322
https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/twin-research-and-human-genetics/article/wilson-effect-the-increase-in-heritability-of-iq-with-age/23A7A40735315579343360A737E17726
https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fa0026699
https://www.nature.com/articles/s41588-018-0147-3
https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797615612727
Intelligence is a complex trait often measured through proxies like IQ tests, which assess cognitive abilities such as reasoning, problem-solving, and memory. The question of how much intelligence is hereditary refers to the concept of heritability—the proportion of variance in intelligence within a population that can be attributed to genetic differences among individuals. Importantly, heritability does not mean that a certain percentage of an individual’s intelligence is “inherited” from parents; rather, it estimates the genetic contribution to differences between people in a specific environment [1]. Estimates of intelligence heritability typically range from 40% to 80%, depending on factors like age, socioeconomic status (SES), and the population studied. This response explores these estimates, the evidence from various studies, and the interplay between genetics and environment.
Heritability is derived from studies comparing genetic relatedness and similarity in intelligence scores. For instance:
A meta-analysis of such studies indicates that heritability of general intelligence (often denoted as g) averages around 50% in adulthood, but it increases with age—from about 30% in childhood to 70-80% in later life. This “Wilson effect” may occur because genetic influences become more pronounced as individuals gain control over their environments [1][3].
However, heritability is not fixed. It can vary by context:
Intelligence is polygenic, meaning it involves thousands of small-effect genetic variants rather than a single “intelligence gene.” Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have identified hundreds of these variants, and polygenic scores derived from them can predict about 10-20% of the variance in intelligence in independent samples [5]. This is lower than overall heritability estimates, suggesting that much of the genetic influence remains “missing” due to rare variants, gene-environment interactions, or limitations in current methods [3].
Epigenetics and gene-environment interplay add complexity. For example, genes may influence how individuals respond to education or stress, amplifying or dampening environmental effects [2].
Even with high heritability, environment plays a crucial role. Non-genetic factors account for the remaining variance (20-60%, depending on the estimate). These include prenatal nutrition, education quality, family environment, and cultural factors. Interventions like early childhood education can boost IQ by 5-10 points, demonstrating malleability [1]. The “Flynn effect”—a 20th-century rise in average IQ scores across generations—highlights environmental impacts, such as improved nutrition and schooling, rather than genetic changes [2].
Critically, heritability does not imply determinism. An individual with a genetic predisposition for high intelligence may underperform in a deprived environment, while supportive conditions can help those with average genetic endowments excel [4].
The heritability of intelligence has been debated, partly due to historical misuse in eugenics or racial pseudoscience. Modern consensus, backed by behavioral genetics, rejects such notions, emphasizing that group differences (e.g., between ethnicities) are largely environmental and that within-group heritability does not explain between-group gaps [1][2]. Measurement issues also arise: IQ tests may not capture all aspects of intelligence (e.g., creativity or emotional intelligence), and cultural biases can affect scores [3].
In summary, while genetics contribute substantially to differences in intelligence—explaining roughly 50% of variance on average—the exact figure varies by context, and environment is equally vital. Ongoing research, including larger GWAS and longitudinal studies, continues to refine these estimates. Understanding this balance underscores the importance of equitable environments to maximize human potential.
[1] Plomin, R., et al. (2016). Top 10 Replicated Findings From Behavioral Genetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science. This source expresses the view that heritability of general intelligence is approximately 50% based on replicated twin and family studies, with genetics and environment interacting. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/1745691615617439
[2] Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three Laws of Behavior Genetics and What They Mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science. The author views heritability as substantial (around 50%) but emphasizes gene-environment interactions and that heritability applies to population variance, not individual causation. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-8721.00084
[3] Bouchard, T. J. (2013). Genetic Influence on Human Psychological Traits. Current Directions in Psychological Science. This review supports high heritability estimates (40-80%) for intelligence, increasing with age, while noting the polygenic nature and missing heritability. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00295.x
[4] Turkheimer, E., et al. (2003). Socioeconomic Status Modifies Heritability of IQ in Young Children. Psychological Science. The authors argue that heritability is lower (about 10%) in low-SES groups and higher (up to 72%) in high-SES groups, highlighting environmental moderation. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9280.02423
[5] Savage, J. E., et al. (2018). Genome-wide Association Meta-analysis in 269,867 Individuals Identifies New Genetic and Functional Links to Intelligence. Nature Genetics. This study finds that polygenic scores explain 5-10% of intelligence variance (with potential up to 20%), supporting a genetic basis but noting gaps in explaining full heritability. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41588-018-0152-6